80069334649J Dev Behav PediatrJ Dev Behav PediatrJournal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics0196-206X1536-731222487695336900010.1097/DBP.0b013e3182564704NIHMS371999ArticleLate-preterm birth by delivery circumstance and its association with parent-reported attention problems in childhoodTalgeNicole M.Ph.D.1HolzmanClaudiaD.V.M, M.P.H., Ph.D.1Van EgerenLaurie A.Ph.D.2SymondsLaura L.Ph.D.3ScheidJeanette M.M.D., Ph.D.4SenagorePatricia K.M.D.5SikorskiiAllaPh.D.6Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824Office of University Outreach and Engagement, Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824College of Human Medicine, Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824Department of Psychiatry, Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824Department of Physiology, Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824Department of Statistics & Probability, Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824Corresponding Author: Nicole M. Talge, Department of Epidemiology, Michigan State University, B601 West Fee Hall, East Lansing, MI 48824. Phone: (517) 353-8623, Fax: (517) 432-1130, ntalge@epi.msu.edu452012620120162013335405415Objective

Late-preterm birth (LPB: 34–36 weeks) has been associated with an increased risk of attention problems in childhood relative to full-term birth (FTB: ≥ 37 weeks), but little is known about factors contributing to this risk. We investigated the contributions of clinical circumstances surrounding delivery using follow-up data from the Pregnancy Outcomes and Community Health (POUCH) Study.

Methods

Women who delivered late-preterm or full-term and completed the sex- and age-referenced Conners’ Parent Rating Scales-Short Form: Revised (CPRS-R:S) were included in the present analysis (N=762: children’s ages: 3–9 years). The CPRS-R:S measures dimensions of behavior linked to attention problems, including: oppositionality, inattention, hyperactivity, and a global attention problem index. Using general linear models, we evaluated whether LPB subtype (medically indicated (MI) or spontaneous) was associated with these dimensions relative to FTB.

Results

After adjustment for parity, socio-demographics, child age, and maternal symptoms of depression and serious mental illness during pregnancy and at the child survey, only MI LPB was associated with higher hyperactivity and global index scores (mean difference from FTB=3.8 [95%CI 0.5,7.0] and 3.1 [95%CI 0.0, 6.2]). These findings were largely driven by children between 6 and 9 years. Removal of women with hypertensive disorders during pregnancy (N=85) or placental findings related to hypertensive conditions (obstruction, decreased maternal spiral artery conversion; N=134) reduced the differences below significance thresholds.

Conclusion

Among LPBs, only MI LPB was associated with higher levels of parent-reported childhood attention problems, suggesting that complications motivating medical intervention during the late-preterm period mark increased risk for such problems. Hypertensive disorders appear to play a role in these associations.

Introduction

From 1998–2008, rates of preterm delivery (< 37 weeks of gestation) increased by 20% in the United States, a trend largely attributed to the rise in deliveries between 34–36 weeks (late-preterm birth (LPB)).1,2 Although many secular trends accompany this pattern (e.g., multifetal gestations), nearly 75% of all singleton preterm deliveries can be accounted for by LPB.1,3 Combined with mounting evidence suggesting that children born late-preterm are at increased risk for neonatal mortality and a range of neonatal morbidities,46 the antecedents and sequelae associated with LPB have received much attention from the medical and public health communities.

LPB has also been associated with long-term cognitive and behavioral problems, including cerebral palsy, reading impairments, lower IQ scores, and higher levels of internalizing and attention-related problems at school-age.712 Retrospective population-based investigations from Europe suggest that adults born late-preterm exhibit elevated risks for psychiatric diagnoses as well as socio-economic hardship,13,14 providing further evidence of long-term impairment among LPBs.13,14 Although little is known about factors contributing to such risks, there is increasing evidence that perinatal health may play a role. For example, children born late-preterm who were admitted to the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) were more likely to exhibit deficits in non-verbal processing in relation to their full-term peers and late-preterm children not admitted to the NICU; no differences between the full-terms and non-NICU admitted LPBs were observed.15 Conversely, while LPB was unrelated to cognitive functioning, behavioral problems, and academic achievement in a longitudinal study spanning childhood and adolescence, analyses excluded pregnancies marked by perinatal health risks (e.g., maternal infection, substance use).16 Delivery circumstance (i.e., medically indicated or spontaneous) may also shed light on LPB associations with long-term child development outcomes,7 but this hypothesis has not been subjected to empirical investigation. Doing so may help identify etiological pathways that may be involved in any associations observed, given that delivery circumstance has been linked to specific indices of pre- and or postnatal complications.17

To address this issue, we used follow-up data from the Pregnancy Outcomes and Community Health (POUCH) Study, a prospective investigation of the etiological pathways leading to preterm delivery. Mothers provided reports of attention problems for the child resulting from the POUCH pregnancy using a norm-referenced instrument. To account for any long-term biological effects related to maternal mental health during pregnancy as well as reporting bias due to such characteristics at the time of the child survey, adjusted analyses incorporated measures of maternal psychosocial functioning obtained at each of these time periods. Our goals were to: 1) replicate previous findings linking LPB to higher levels of attention problems in childhood, and 2) examine whether delivery circumstances surrounding LPB (i.e., MI, spontaneous) were associated with these problems. In the event that significant associations were observed, we utilized additional medical record information and placental histology findings to investigate pathways that might be involved.

Methods<sec id="S8" sec-type="subjects"><title>Participants

The POUCH Study, designed to examine etiological pathways leading to preterm delivery, enrolled 3019 women (15–27 weeks of gestation) from 52 prenatal clinics in five Michigan communities from 1998–2004.18 Eligibility criteria included English proficiency and a singleton pregnancy with no known birth defects, chromosomal anomalies, or preexisting diabetes. All interested, eligible women with maternal serum alpha-fetoprotein (MSAFP) ≥ 2.0 multiples of the median (MoM) and a race-stratified sample of women with MSAFP < 2.0 MoM were included in the POUCH cohort. For a subset oversampled for African-American race, high MSAFP, and preterm delivery (i.e., subcohort: N=1371), in-depth medical record abstraction was performed and placental samples were collected. This sampling scheme was employed to maximize resources when investigating at-risk subgroups,18 but we excluded early preterm deliveries (< 34 weeks, N=68) here given the topic at hand. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Michigan State University and participating medical centers.

In 2007–2008, women were invited to complete a survey about their health and that of the child born during the POUCH Study (i.e., POUCHchild Survey). Exclusion criteria included women who: 1) declined participation in future studies (N=70), 2) did not have legal custody of the POUCH child (N=10), or 3) were deceased or whose child was deceased (N=12), yielding 1211 subcohort women eligible for participation. Of these, 780 (64%) completed the POUCHchild Survey.

To preserve the fidelity of attention problem measurement, children whose mothers reported that they were diagnosed with mental retardation or an autism spectrum disorder (N=12), or were less than 3 years of age at the time of the survey (N=6) were excluded prior to analysis. Thus, the sample size for the present study is 762 with children’s ages ranging between 3 and 9 years (Late-preterm (34–36 weeks): N=152; Full-term (≥ 37 weeks): N=610) (Figure 1).

MeasuresPregnancy Outcomes

Gestational age was estimated using last menstrual period unless it was unavailable or differed from the ultrasound estimate (at < 25 weeks) by more than two weeks. In these cases (20% of the subcohort; 15% of the analytic sample), the ultrasound-based estimate was employed. Medical records were independently abstracted by a physician and labor and delivery nurse, and two categories of preterm delivery circumstances were described: 1) spontaneous: women with regular contractions that led to cervical changes (≥ 2 cm of dilatation) or rupture of membranes before or simultaneously with the onset of contractions, and 2) medically indicated: women induced or given caesarean sections before either preterm labor or rupture of membranes. The source of gestational age estimation was unrelated to pregnancy outcome (data not shown).

Using gestational age, sex, and birthweight information abstracted from medical records, children were identified as small for gestational-age (SGA) using estimates corresponding to the 10th percentile.19

Attention Problems

Mothers completed the Conners’ Parent Rating Scales-Revised: Short Form (CPRS-R:S), an instrument yielding sex- and age-referenced T-scores for three dimensions of behavior associated with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), including: oppositionality (irritability, anger-proneness, and defiance), cognitive problems/inattention (difficulty concentrating, planning, or maintaining attention on tasks requiring sustained mental effort), and hyperactivity (impulsivity and restlessness).20 The CPRS-R:S also yields a global ADHD Index, which measures elements of both inattention and hyperactivity. T-scores were normally distributed and not transformed prior to analysis. The CPRS -R:S is normed for 3–17 years olds and exhibits discriminate validity. That is, children diagnosed attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) score significantly higher on each of the CPRS-R:S scales compared to non-clinical samples as well as children exhibiting other types of psychopathologies (e.g., anxiety disorders).20

Maternal Psychosocial Functioning

At POUCH Study enrollment, women completed the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D).21 Women were grouped according to whether their total scores were equal to or above 24, a threshold that has been associated with adverse perinatal outcomes.22 At POUCHchild Survey completion, women completed the Kessler Scales,23 a validated instrument designed to screen for serious mental illnesses (SMIs) in the general population. SMIs are mental, behavioral, and emotional disorders associated with functional impairment in daily activities. Using thresholds described in previous research,23 total scores equal to or above 19 were defined as high.

Hypertensive Disorders

Evidence of hypertensive disorders (i.e., chronic hypertension (CH), gestational hypertension (GH), preeclampsia (PE)) was abstracted from medical charts. Diagnostic categories included: CH (diastolic blood pressure (DBP) > 90 or systolic blood pressure (SBP) > 140 on at least two occasions prior to 20 weeks, medical record diagnosis, or use of anti-hypertensive medication), GH (no CH, DBP > 90 or SBP > 140 on at least two occasions after 20 weeks), and PE (same criteria as GH plus evidence of proteinuria).2426

Placental Pathology

Placentas were obtained from 88% of the subcohort and, to date, are available for 85% of women included in the present analysis (N=649). Nine samples per placenta (placental disc (5), membrane roll (2), umbilical cord (2)) were examined by the study pathologist blinded to all clinical circumstances surrounding delivery. Microscopic evidence of vascular pathology was evaluated and grouped into five constructs, detailed elsewhere.27 Here, we focus on two constructs that have been linked to the presence of hypertensive disorders during pregnancy: Maternal Vascular–Obstructive (MV-O) (e.g., infarcts, decidual vessel atherosis), and Maternal Vascular–Developmental (MV-D) (e.g., abnormal/incomplete conversion of the uterine spiral arteries).28,29 To maximize power in data analysis, women were grouped according to whether they exhibited high levels of pathology in either the MV-O or MV-D construct.27

Covariates

Maternal self-reports of race (non-Hispanic white/other, African-American), education (≤ 12, 12 years), age (<25, ≥ 25 years), parity (primiparous, multiparous), and tobacco use (none, any) were obtained at POUCH Study enrollment. Child age was obtained at the time of the POUCHchild Survey.

Statistical Analysis

All analyses, both unadjusted and adjusted, were weighted for the over-sampling of women with high MSAFP into the cohort and high MSAFP, African-American race, and preterm delivery into the subcohort (see Participants). That is, these subsets of participants were given less weight in analyses because they were oversampled into the POUCH cohort and subcohort. All covariates noted above were included in the adjusted models along with indices of maternal psychosocial functioning.

Associations between LPB and attention problems were evaluated in three steps. First, to better contextualize any findings observed, general linear models (GLMs) comparing characteristics of the POUCH subcohort and the analytic sample were performed. In an attempt to replicate previous research findings, a second set of GLMs evaluated whether LPB, relative to FTB, was associated with higher levels of attention problems. Finally, GLMs were used to evaluate whether LPB circumstance was associated with attention problems relative to FTB and whether child sex or maternal symptoms of SMIs modified any observed findings. Due to concerns regarding the validity of assessments of attention problems at preschool-age,30 this final set of analyses were stratified by child age at survey (3 to < 6 years: N = 484; 6–9 years: N =278) to better contextualize any findings observed.

Because MI LPB was significantly associated with child attention problems (see Results/Table 3) and hypertensive conditions have been implicated in MI LPBs,3,28,31 the final models were repeated after removing women with indicators linked to hypertensive disorders during pregnancy (e.g., high MV-O/MV-D or SGA birth) to see if such factors partially or fully accounted for the findings. Because indices of inflammatory conditions and bleeding have also been associated with LPB,32,33 we repeated analyses following the removal of women with severe histologic chorioamnionitis (HCA) or placental abruption to further probe the specificity of any observed associations. Small cell sizes precluded the removal of these subgroups in age-stratified analyses.

Results

Relative to the subcohort, participation in the follow-up study and inclusion in the analytic sample was significantly associated with older maternal age, higher levels of maternal education, and married marital status at POUCH Study enrollment and older child age at POUCHChild Study participation (Table 1). Pregnancy outcomes and their association with maternal, child, and placental characteristics in the analytic sample are summarized in Table 2. Compared to FTB, MI LPB was associated with African-American race, diagnosis of hypertensive disorders during pregnancy, and SGA birth. No covariates were associated with spontaneous LPB.

In both the unadjusted and adjusted analyses, LPB collapsed across delivery circumstance was not associated with any aspect of parent-reported attention problems relative to FTB (unadjusted mean differences: oppositionality: 1.1 [95%CI −1.3,3.5]; inattention: 0.5 [95% CI −1.5,2.5]; hyperactivity: 0.7 [95%CI −1.5,2.9]; ADHD Index: −.10 [95%CI −0.3,0.1]) (Table 3).

When grouped by delivery circumstance, MI LPB was associated with higher levels of hyperactivity and tended to be associated with higher ADHD Index scores relative to FTB in unadjusted analyses (mean differences: 3.9 [95%CI .5,7.3] and 3.0 [95%CI −0.3,6.3], respectively) (Table 3). These findings were replicated following adjustment for potential confounders, except that the association between MI LPB and the ADHD Index exceeded significance thresholds (mean differences: 3.8 [95%CI 0.6,7.2] and 3.1 [95%CI 0.0,6.1]); findings corresponded to effect sizes (Cohen’s d) between .3 and .4, a range often interpreted as moderate in magnitude.34 We found no evidence that associations were modified by child sex or maternal symptoms of SMIs at the time of the child survey. Findings were similarly unaffected when birth weight z-scores, maternal pre-pregnancy body mass index, or multiparity between POUCH Study enrollment and POUCHChild Survey participation were included in the multivariate models (data not shown). With the exception of the inattention subscale, MI LPB was not associated with any CPRS dimension among the preschool-aged children. However, MI LPB was significantly associated with the inattention, hyperactivity, and the ADHD Index among the 6–9 year olds (Table 4); the size of these effects was considerable, with d’s ranging from .5–.6. No outliers were detected in these age-stratified analyses.

Because hypertensive conditions have been linked to MI LPB,27,31 analyses were repeated excluding women with clinical diagnoses of hypertensive disorders (N=85) or placental findings linked to these conditions (N=134), and were performed on the entire analytic sample to maximize cell sizes (see Methods). In each of the analyses (Table 5), the unadjusted and adjusted associations of MI LPB with hyperactivity and ADHD Index scores were no longer significant; effect sizes also decreased (d=.06–.25). Removal of SGAs reduced associations between MI LPB and ADHD Index scores to non-significance and attenuated the associations between MI LPB and hyperactivity to trend levels (unadjusted mean difference: 3.6 [95%CI −0.3,7.3]). In contrast, associations of MI LPB with hyperactivity and ADHD Index scores were maintained when women exhibiting severe levels of histologic chorioamnionitis35 (HCA) (N=63) or placental abruption36 (N=16) were removed from analysis (data not shown).

Discussion

On average, children born late-preterm did not exhibit higher levels of parent-reported attention problems compared to those born full-term. However, when LPB was grouped by delivery circumstance, MI LPB was associated with higher levels of hyperactivity and higher scores on a composite marking inattentive and hyperactivity symptoms. Following removal of women with hypertensive disorders or placental findings associated with hypertensive conditions, these findings were attenuated and no longer significant, suggesting that hypertensive disorders may play a role in the associations.

Delivery during the late-preterm period has received considerable epidemiologic attention. To date, conditions linked to LPB include infections (e.g., HCA)33 as well as hypertensive disorders, fetal growth restriction, and placental abruption;3739 this latter group of conditions has been linked to deliveries prompted by medical intervention.37,40 Despite the range of factors associated with LPB, we provide evidence suggesting that diagnoses of hypertensive conditions as well as placental findings implicated in these conditions contribute to the associations reported here. Indeed, removal of women with placental abruption or who exhibit severe HCA did not affect any of our findings. Although removal of SGA births did slightly attenuate links between MI LPB and hyperactivity symptoms, this group likely includes children who are constitutionally small and may not represent the most sensitive approach to identifying fetal growth restriction. Hypertensive conditions during pregnancy, as well as the placental vascular findings investigated here, may mark disturbances in placental perfusion which in turn impede gas, waste, and nutrient exchange between mother and fetus;29 links between hypertensive disorders and alterations in placental hormone secretion have also been observed (e.g., corticotropin-releasing hormone).4143 The extent to which such biological alterations affect brain development, particularly circuits implicated in the development of attention problems (e.g., frontal-striatal and cerebellar networks), require further elucidation.4447

Interestingly, studies investigating links between hypertensive conditions and the development of attention problems have generally yielded null results.4850 There may be several factors contributing to this apparent contradiction to our findings, including a relative lack of socio-economic and racial/ethnic diversity in the study samples,49 and more importantly, grouping preterms and terms together when evaluating these associations.50 To the extent that the threshold motivating medical intervention changes across gestational age, such approaches may obscure associations that are due to symptom severity and may be revealed by testing for effect modification by gestational age at delivery. Many studies also employ composite indices of perinatal risk that represent a range of obstetric and neonatal complications,49,5154 thus obscuring associations between any given condition and the outcome of interest.

Nonetheless, our study findings may be relevant to outstanding issues in the LPB outcome literature. Although delivery prior to 34 weeks is more strongly associated with a range of developmental outcomes, including long-term behavioral problems,55,56 we provide evidence suggesting that links between MI LPB and attention problems may be clinically-meaningful (0.3<d<0.4). Additionally, previous investigations suggest that only a subset of children born late-preterm appear to be at-risk for later attention problems.7,15 Our study extends these findings by identifying maternal hypertensive disorders as one potential source/marker of such heterogeneity.

In contrast to other investigations, we did not observe an association between LPB and attention problems when collapsing across delivery circumstance. There may be several reasons for this. First, the majority of children in our study were younger than those in studies documenting associations between LPB and attention-related problems; to the extent that main effects of LPB are more readily observed at older ages, especially with respect to attention problems (next paragraph), this might have interfered with our ability to detect such differences across the LPB subgroups. Additionally, other studies documenting associations between LPB and attention problems might reflect the characteristics of the samples from which they were drawn. For example, links between LPB and attention problems may be more readily apparent in samples enriched with children who were growth restricted at birth or experienced neonatal complications.7,15 Providing some support for this hypothesis, analyses utilizing NICHD Early Child Care data found no differences between LPB and FTB in terms of behavioral problems,16 but children from complicated pregnancies were excluded from study.

There are several caveats to consider when interpreting our findings. First, although we employed a norm-referenced measure of attention problems, the main outcome of this study was parent-reported. We therefore cannot rule out that maternal knowledge of children’s PTB status influenced their reports of attention problems. However, because our findings were limited to MI LPB, it is unlikely this issue explains the study findings. Additionally, longitudinal investigations of parenting quality suggest that while there may be alterations in maternal behavior during the first year of life as a function of PTB status, such differences are not observed in toddlerhood when measured using the Strange Situation, a lab-based observation considered to be the gold standard for characterizing parent-child relationship quality at that age.5759 Second, measures of socio-economic and demographic factors used in this report were obtained at POUCH Study enrollment. Changes in family structure and/or income over time may contribute to individual differences in behavioral problems in children,6063 and we were unable to investigate such effects or otherwise contextualize findings with contemporaneous socio-economic indicators. Third, although our analyses were drawn from a community sample, some were limited in power due to the relatively small number of MI LPBs. This is particularly true with respect to the age-stratified analyses as well as those involving the removal of subgroups based upon placental or medical record information. Given these concerns, we chose not to adjust for multiple comparisons. While we believe the consistency of findings across analyses helps assuage concerns regarding Type I error rate, replication of our findings is warranted and encouraged. Our analyses also assumed that non-missing data from a particular weighted sampling stratum were representative of that stratum. To address this assumption, we performed unweighted analyses and observed no alterations to the pattern of findings reported here. Caution should nonetheless be employed when considering the generalizability of the study findings, particularly given the follow-up rate for eligible LPBs (54%). Combined with evidence suggesting that POUCHChild Survey participation was associated with higher socio-economic status indicators (see Results), the current analysis may not reflect the full-spectrum of LPBs. Finally, it is important to note that MI LPB is a risk factor for subsequent neonatal morbidity,64 and as such, this study is unable to identify whether prenatal, postnatal, or some combination of these factors contribute to the outcomes observed here.

Despite these caveats, there are notable strengths of this investigation, including the prospective design and demographic breadth from which the analytic sample was drawn. Additionally, the availability of detailed medical record information and placental findings allowed for an investigation of factors that may underlie the associations reported here. Information regarding maternal psychosocial functioning during pregnancy and at child survey completion helped address concerns that co-occurrences between maternal and child psychopathology contributed to the study findings.65 Because there is no indication that women with attention problems are more likely to exhibit MI LPBs, we do not believe that other factors contributing to such co-occurrence fully account for the findings here (e.g., heritability).

This study provides many directions for future investigation. In addition to replicating the current findings, the extent to which associations between hypertension-associated LPB and attention problems persist across development requires elucidation. Additionally, LPB has been associated with other types of behavioral problems in childhood besides attention problems (e.g., anxious/depressive symptoms),7,13 but at present, we have very little understanding of what factors contribute to the different types of outcomes observed among these children. This latter issue underscores the need to better understand the mechanisms involved in associations reported here and elsewhere.

In sum, we provide evidence suggesting that MI LPB is associated with higher levels of attention problems in childhood. Given that previous investigations point to marked heterogeneity in outcomes of children born late-preterm, studies such as ours may help identify children at highest risk for later difficulties while at the same time, uncover the mechanisms involved.

This study was supported by the Perinatal Epidemiological Research Initiative Program [Grant 20-FY04-37] from the March of Dimes Foundation, the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development and the National Institute of Nursing Research [Grant R01 HD34543], the Thrasher Research Foundation [Grant 02816-7], and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [Grant U01 DP000143-01] to Claudia Holzman. The principal investigator of the POUCH Study (Claudia Holzman) had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis

CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: This study was supported by the Perinatal Epidemiological Research Initiative Program [Grant 20-FY04-37] from the March of Dimes Foundation, the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development and the National Institute of Nursing Research [Grant R01 HD34543], the Thrasher Research Foundation [Grant 02816-7], and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [Grant U01 DP000143-01] to Claudia Holzman.

ABBREVIATIONSADHD

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

CES-D

Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale

CPRS-R: S

Conners’ Parent Report-Revised: Short Form

GLM

general linear model

HCA

histologic chorioamnionitis

IQ

intelligence quotient

LPB

late preterm birth

MI

medically indicated

MoM

multiples of the median

MSAFP

maternal serum alpha fetoprotein

MV-D

maternal vascular-developmental

MV-O

maternal vascular-obstructive

NICHD

National Institute of Child Health and Human Development

NICU

neonatal intensive care unit

PE

preeclampsia

POUCH Study

Pregnancy Outcome and Community Health Study

PTD

preterm delivery

SGA

small for gestational age

SMIs

Serious Mental Illnesses

MartinJAKirmeyerSOstermanMShepherdRABorn a bit too early: recent trends in late preterm birthsNCHS Data Brief112009241819922725MartinJAHamiltonBESuttonPDVenturaSJMenackerFMunsonMLBirths: final data for 2003Natl Vital Stat Rep9820055421116DavidoffMJDiasTDamusKChanges in the gestational age distribution among U.S. singleton births: impact on rates of late preterm birth, 1992 to 2002Semin Perinatol2200630181516549207KramerMSLate preterm birth: appreciable risks, rising incidenceJ Pediatr22009154215916019081112Shapiro-MendozaCKTomashekKMKotelchuckMEffect of late-preterm birth and maternal medical conditions on newborn morbidity riskPediatrics220081212e22323218245397TomashekKMShapiro-MendozaCKDavidoffMJPetriniJRDifferences in mortality between late-preterm and term singleton infants in the United States, 1995–2002J Pediatr1120071515450456456e45117961684TalgeNMHolzmanCWangJLuciaVGardinerJBreslauNLate-preterm birth and its association with cognitive and socioemotional outcomes at 6 years of agePediatrics12201012661124113121098151NomuraYRajendranKBrooks-GunnJNewcornJHRoles of perinatal problems on adolescent antisocial behaviors among children born after 33 completed weeks: a prospective investigationJ Child Psychol Psychiatry10200849101108111718673404NomuraYHalperinJMNewcornJHThe risk for impaired learning-related abilities in childhood and educational attainment among adults born near-termJ Pediatr Psychol5200934440641818794190PetriniJRDiasTMcCormickMCMassoloMLGreenNSEscobarGJIncreased risk of adverse neurological development for late preterm infantsJ Pediatr22009154216917619081113LinnetKMWisborgKAgerboESecherNJThomsenPHHenriksenTBGestational age, birth weight, and the risk of hyperkinetic disorderArch Dis Child8200691865566016754656MorseSBZhengHTangYRothJEarly school-age outcomes of late preterm infantsPediatrics420091234e62262919336353LindstromKLindbladFHjernAPsychiatric morbidity in adolescents and young adults born preterm: a Swedish national cohort studyPediatrics120091231e475319117846MosterDLieRTMarkestadTLong-term medical and social consequences of preterm birthN Engl J Med7172008359326227318635431BaronISEricksonKAhronovichMDBakerRLitmanFRCognitive deficit in preschoolers born late-pretermEarly Hum Dev2201187211511921131147GurkaMJLoCasale-CrouchJBlackmanJALong-term cognition, achievement, socioemotional, and behavioral development of healthy late-preterm infantsArch Pediatr Adolesc Med62010164652553220530302BerkowitzGSBlackmore-PrinceCLapinskiRHSavitzDARisk factors for preterm birth subtypesEpidemiology51998932792859583419HolzmanCBullenBFisherRPanethNReussLPregnancy outcomes and community health: The POUCH study of preterm deliveryPaediatr Perinat Ep200115136158AlexanderGRHimesJHKaufmanRBMorJKoganMA United States national reference for fetal growthObstet Gynecol1996871631688559516ConnersCKConners’ rating scales-revised (CRS-R) technical manualTonawanda, N.YMultihealth Systems2001RadloffLThe CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general populationApplied Psychological Measurement197713385401GavinARHolzmanCSiefertKTianYMaternal depressive symptoms, depression, and psychiatric medication use in relation to risk of preterm deliveryWomens Health IssuesSep-Oct200919532533419733802KesslerRCAndrewsGColpeLJShort Screening Scales to Monitor Population Prevalences and Trends in Non-specific Psychological DistressPsychol Med20023295997612214795LevineRJHauthJCCuretLBTrial of calcium to prevent pre-eclampsiaNew Eng J Med1997337269779211675ZambrowskiNHBPEP report on high blood pressure in pregnancy: A summary for family physiciansAm Fam Physician2001642264Report of the National High Blood Pressure Education Program Working Group on high blood pressure in pregnancyAm J Obstet Gynecol20001831S1S22KellyRHolzmanCSenagorePPlacental vascular pathology findings and pathways to preterm deliveryAm J Epidemiol7152009170214815819509320SalafiaCMErnstLMPezzulloJCWolfEJRosenkrantzTSVintzileosAMThe very low birthweight infant: maternal complications leading to preterm birth, placental lesions, and intrauterine growthAm J Perinatol319951221061107779189BaergenRNManual of Benirschke and Kaufmann’s Pathology of the Human PlacentaNew YorkSpringer2005WolraichMBrownLBrownRTADHD: clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescentsPediatrics11201112851007102222003063CarrenoCACostantineMMHollandMGRaminSMSaadeGRBlackwellSCApproximately one-third of medically indicated late preterm births are complicated by fetal growth restrictionAm J Obstet Gynecol112in pressMcIntireDDLevenoKJNeonatal mortality and morbidity rates in late preterm births compared with births at termObstet Gynecol120081111354118165390KhashuMNarayananMBhargavaSOsiovichHPerinatal outcomes associated with preterm birth at 33 to 36 weeks’ gestation: a population-based cohort studyPediatrics12009123110911319117868CohenJStatistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences2Hillsdale Lawrence Erlbaum1988HolzmanCLinXSenagorePChungHHistologic chorioamnionitis and preterm deliveryAm J Epidemiol1012007166778679417625222GarganoJWHolzmanCBSenagorePKPolymorphisms in thrombophilia and renin-angiotensin system pathways, preterm delivery, and evidence of placental hemorrhageAm J Obstet Gynecol920092013317e31131919733287FuchsKGyamfiCThe influence of obstetric practices on late prematurityClin Perinatol62008352343360vi18456073DobakWJGardnerMOLate preterm gestation: physiology of labor and implications for deliveryClin Perinatol122006334765776abstract vii17148003SibaiBMPreeclampsia as a cause of preterm and late preterm (near-term) birthsSemin Perinatol22006301161916549208HollandMGRefuerzoJSRaminSMSaadeGRBlackwellSCLate preterm birth: how often is it avoidable?Am J Obstet Gynecol1020092014404e40140419716546GolandRSConwellIMJozakSThe effect of pre-eclampsia on human placental corticotrophin-releasing hormone content and processingPlacenta619951643753827567800AhmedIGlynnBPPerkinsAVProcessing of procorticotropin-releasing hormone (pro-CRH): molecular forms of CRH in normal and preeclamptic pregnancyJ Clin Endocrinol Metab2200085275576410690887LiapiCATsakaliaDEPanitsa-FafliaCCAntsaklisAIAravantinosDIBatrinosMLCorticotropin-releasing-hormone levels in pregnancy-induced hypertensionEur J Obstet Gynecol Reprod Biol91996681–21091148886691KonradKEickhoffSBIs the ADHD brain wired differently? A review on structural and functional connectivity in attention deficit hyperactivity disorderHum Brain Mapp6201031690491620496381TianLJiangTWangYAltered resting-state functional connectivity patterns of anterior cingulate cortex in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorderNeurosci Lett52920064001–2394316510242CastellanosFXMarguliesDSKellyCCingulate-precuneus interactions: a new locus of dysfunction in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorderBiol Psychiatry21200863333233717888409RubiaKHalariRCubilloAMohammadAMBrammerMTaylorEMethylphenidate normalises activation and functional connectivity deficits in attention and motivation networks in medication-naive children with ADHD during a rewarded continuous performance taskNeuropharmacology122009577–864065219715709NiggJTNikolasMBSAMeasured gene-by-environment interaction in relation to attention deficit/hyperactivity disorderJ Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry20104986387320732623WagnerAISchmidtNLLemery-ChalfantKLeavittLAGoldsmithHHThe limited effects of obstetrical and neonatal complications on conduct and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in middle childhoodJ Dev Behav Pediatr6200930321722519433988GustafssonPKallenKPerinatal, maternal, and fetal characteristics of children diagnosed with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder: results from a population-based study utilizing the Swedish Medical Birth RegisterDev Med Child Neurol3201053326326820964677CurtisWJLindekeLLGeorgieffMKNelsonCANeurobehavioural functioning in neonatal intensive care unit graduates in late childhood and early adolescenceBrain72002125Pt 71646165912077013LucianaMLindekeLGeorgieffMMillsMNelsonCANeurobehavioral evidence for working-memory deficits in school-aged children with histories of prematurityDev Med Child Neurol8199941852153310479041Ben AmorLGrizenkoNSchwartzGPerinatal complications in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblingsJ Psychiatry Neurosci3200530212012615798787AllenNBLewinsohnPMSeeleyJRPrenatal and perinatal influences on risk for psychopathology in childhood and adolescenceDev PsychopatholSummer19981035135299741680LindstromKLindbladFHjernAPreterm birth and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in schoolchildrenPediatrics52011127585886521502231SchothorstPFvan EngelandHLong-term behavioral sequelae of prematurityJ Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry219963521751838720627FrodiAThompsonRInfants’ affective responses in the strange situation: effects of prematurity and of quality of attachmentChild Dev101985565128012904053743MilesMSHolditch-DavisDParenting the prematurely born child: pathways of influenceSemin Perinatol619972132542669205979EasterbrooksMAQuality of attachment to mother and to father: Effects of perinatal risk statusChild Dev198960825830WeinfieldNSWhaleyGJEgelandBContinuity, discontinuity, and coherence in attachment from infancy to late adolescence: sequelae of organization and disorganizationAttach Hum Dev3200461739714982680WeinfieldNSSroufeLAEgelandBAttachment from infancy to early adulthood in a high-risk sample: continuity, discontinuity, and their correlatesChild DevMay-Jun200071369570210953936SroufeLAAttachment and development: a prospective, longitudinal study from birth to adulthoodAttach Hum Dev1220057434936716332580UrbanJCarlsonEEgelandBSroufeLAPatterns of individual adaptation across childhoodDev Psychopathol19913445460GouyonJBVintejouxASagotPBurguetAQuantinCFerdynusCNeonatal outcome associated with singleton birth at 34–41 weeks of gestationInt J Epidemiol6201039376977620304783ChilcoatHDBreslauNDoes psychiatric history bias mothers’ reports? An application of a new analytic approachJ Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry719973679719799204676

Analytic Sample Derivation

Maternal, child, and placental characteristics of the POUCH subcohort and the analytic samplea

POUCH Subcohort (N = 1303)b
Analytic Sample (N = 762)
N(Wt %)N(Wt %)
Maternal Characteristics
Age (years)*
 < 25622(43)322(39)
 ≥ 25681(57)440(61)
Education (years)*
 ≤ 12672(46)329(41)
 > 12631(54)433(59)
Race
 White/Other754(76)504(76)
 African-American549(24)258(24)
Parity
 Primiparous544(41)325(42)
 Multiparous758(59)437(58)
Marital Status*
 Unmarried750(48)385(46)
 Married549(52)377(54)
Tobacco Use
 No1054(82)637(84)
 Yes249(18)125(16)
Hypertensive Disorders
 No1168(91)677(90)
 Yes135(9)85(10)
Depressive Symptoms during Pregnancy
 CES-D < 241049(84)641(86)
 CES-D ≥ 24249(16)119(14)
Serious Mental Illness Symptoms: POUCHChild Survey
 K6 < 19681(90)
 K6 ≥ 1981(10)
Child Characteristics
Small for Gestational Age
 No1158(91)689(91)
 Yes144(9)73(9)
Child Sex
 Male668(49)386(48)
 Female635(51)376(52)
Child Age at Survey*
 3 to < 6 years1019(71)484(61)
 6 to 9 years284(29)278(39)
Placental Characteristics
High MV-O or MV-DN = 1064N = 649
 No836(80)515(80)
 Yes228(20)134(20)

Abbreviations: CES-D (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale); K6 (Kessler Screening Instrument for Serious Mental Illnesses); MV-D (Maternal Vascular Pathology: Developmental); MV-O (Maternal Vascular Pathology: Obstructive)

Note. Missing data from POUCH subcohort: Parity (N=1), Marital Status (N=4), Depressive Symptom during Pregnancy (N=5), Small for Gestational Age (N=1). Missing data from analytic sample: Depressive Symptoms during Pregnancy (N=2).

Weighted percents reflect the removal of early preterm deliveries (< 34 weeks) from the POUCH cohort (and thus, the subcohort) prior to adjustment for the POUCH Study sampling scheme.

Sample size reflects removal of early preterm deliveries (N=68)

p < 0.05

Maternal, child, and placental characteristics in relation to pregnancy outcome (N=762) a

Full TermN=610
Late-Preterm Circumstance (N=152)
Medically IndicatedN = 38
SpontaneousN = 114
N(Wt %)N(Wt %)N(Wt %)
Maternal Characteristics
Age (years)
 < 25256(36)14(37)52(45)
 ≥ 25354(64)24(63)62(55)
Education (years)
 ≤ 12259(38)15(38)55(48)
 > 12351(62)23(62)59(52)
Race*
 White/Other390(83)28(73)86(77)
 African-American220(17)10(27)28(23)
Parity
 Primiparous250(42)17(46)58(51)
 Multiparous360(58)21(54)56(49)
Marital Status
 Unmarried308(41)19(49)58(50)
 Married302(59)19(51)56(50)
Tobacco Use
 No510(85)36(95)91(81)
 Yes100(15)2(5)23(19)
Hypertensive Disorders*
 No551(90)21(54)105(92)
 Yes59(10)17(45)9(8)
Depressive Symptoms during Pregnancy
 CES-D < 24511(87)36(95)94(82)
 CES-D ≥ 2497(13)2(5)20(18)
Serious Mental Illness Symptoms: POUCHChild Survey
 K6 < 19545(90)33(87)103(90)
 K6 ≥ 1965(10)5(13)11(10)
Child Characteristics
Small for Gestational Age*
 No552(92)29(77)108(94)
 Yes58(8)9(23)6(6)
Child Sex
 Male305(47)16(42)65(55)
 Female305(53)22(58)49(45)
Child Age at Survey
 3 to < 6 years388(60)22(57)74(64)
 6 to 9 years222(40)16(43)40(36)
Placental Characteristics
High MV-O or MV-DN = 510N = 36N = 103
 No407(81)25(70)83(80)
 Yes103(19)11(30)20(20)

Abbreviations: CES-D (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale); K6 (Kessler Screening Instrument for Serious Mental Illnesses); MV-D (Maternal Vascular Pathology: Developmental); MV-O (Maternal Vascular Pathology: Obstructive)

Note. Missing data: Depressive Symptoms during Pregnancy (N=2)

Analytic sample weighted to reflect the POUCH Subcohort

p < 0.05 relative to full-term group

Unadjusted and adjusted associations between pregnancy outcome and parent-reported attention problemsa,b

Full Term (referent)N = 610
Late-PretermN = 152
Late-Preterm Circumstance (N=152)
Medically IndicatedN=38
SpontaneousN = 114
Mean[95%CI]Mean[95%CI]Mean[95%CI]Mean[95%CI]
Oppositionality
 unadjusted51.5[50.5, 52.5]52.6[50.8, 54.4]53.6[50.0, 57.6]52.2[50.1, 54.3]
 adjusted57.7[55.5, 60.1]58.6[55.7, 61.6]60.7[55.8, 66.0]57.9[54.9, 61.0]
Inattention
 unadjusted51.5[50.7, 52.4]52.0[50.4, 53.5]52.4[49.6, 55.4]51.8[50.0, 53.7]
 adjusted56.1[54.0, 58.3]56.6[54.1, 59.2]57.0[53.2, 61.0]56.5[53.8, 59.3]
Hyperactivity
 unadjusted54.9[53.9, 56.0]55.6[53.8, 57.3]58.8*[55.5, 62.0]54.5[52.4, 56.5]
 adjusted62.5[60.0, 65.0]62.7[59.9, 65.6]66.3*[62.3, 70.3]61.5[58.6, 64.5]
ADHD Index
 unadjusted53.2[52.3, 54.1]53.1[51.5, 54.8]56.2[53.1, 59.4]52.1[50.2, 53.9]
 adjusted59.5[57.4, 61.5]59.3[56.9, 61.8]62.6*[59.0, 66.2]58.2[55.6, 60.8]

Weighting applied so that the analytic sample reflects the POUCH Subcohort.

Adjusted for: maternal race, education, marital status, parity, maternal age, tobacco use, and depressive symptoms during pregnancy, symptoms of serious mental illness at time of child survey, child sex, and child age at survey

p < 0.05 relative to full-term group

Adjusted associations between pregnancy outcome and parent-reported attention problems stratified by child age (3 to <6 years: N = 484; 6–9 years: N = 278) a,b

Full Term (referent)Late-PretermLate-Preterm Circumstance
Medically IndicatedSpontaneous
3 to < 6 years: N = 3886 – 9 years: N = 222
3 to < 6 years: N = 966 – 9 years: N = 56
3 to < 6 years: N = 226 – 9 years: N = 16
3 to < 6 years: N = 746 – 9 years: N = 40
Mean[95%CI]Mean[95%CI]Mean[95%CI]Mean[95%CI]
Oppositionality
 3 to < 6 years57.3[54.7, 59.9]57.4[54.7, 59.9]57.2[52.2, 62.7]57.5[53.9, 61.4]
 6 to 9 years58.1[53.8, 62.7]60.4[55.1, 66.3]64.8[55.4, 75.8]58.8[53.6, 64.5]
Inattention
 3 to < 6 years56.8[54.4, 59.3]57.1[54.2, 60.1]53.2*[50.1, 56.5]58.3[55.0, 66.8]
 6 to 9 years55.4[51.3, 59.8]57.5[52.6, 63.0]63.4*[55.9, 71.9]55.4[50.2, 61.1]
Hyperactivity
 3 to < 6 years62.5[59.7, 65.3]62.6[59.3, 65.9]63.8[59.1, 68.6]62.1[58.6, 65.8]
 6 to 9 years62.9[58.4, 67.4]64.4[59.3, 69.5]70.3*[63.8, 76.8]62.2[56.7, 67.4]
ADHD Index
 3 to < 6 years58.9[56.7, 61.1]58.3[55.5, 61.1]58.3[54.4, 61.2]58.8[55.2, 61.4]
 6 to 9 years60.2[56.3, 64.1]61.8[57.2, 66.4]68.1*[62.2, 74.0]59.4[54.5, 64.3]

Weighting applied so that the analytic sample reflects the POUCH Subcohort.

Adjusted for: maternal race, education, marital status, parity, maternal age, tobacco use, and depressive symptoms during pregnancy, symptoms of serious mental illness at time of child survey, and child sex.

p < 0.05 relative to full-term group

Unadjusted and adjusted associations between pregnancy outcome and attention problems following the removal of women with hypertensive disorders, high placental MV-O or MV-D, or small for gestational age births a,b

Full Term (referent) N = 610
Late-Preterm Circumstance (N=152)
Medically IndicatedN = 38
SpontaneousN = 114
Mean[95%CI]Mean[95%CI]Mean[95%CI]
No Hypertensive DisordersN = 551N = 21N = 105
Oppositionality
  unadjusted51.6[50.5, 52.7]49.5[46.5, 52.7]52.3[50.1, 54.5]
  adjusted57.2[54.8, 59.6]54.7[50.5, 59.2]57.5[54.5, 60.8]
Inattention
  unadjusted51.6[50.7, 52.6]51.5[47.3, 55.9]52.1[50.1, 54.1]
  adjusted56.2[54.0, 58.5]55.8[50.7, 61.4]56.8[53.9, 59.8]
Hyperactivity
  unadjusted55.1[53.9, 56.2]56.0[51.8, 60.2]54.6[52.4, 56.9]
  adjusted62.4[59.8, 64.9]62.7[58.2, 67.3]61.5[58.4, 64.6]
ADHD Index
  unadjusted53.2[52.3, 54.2]53.6[48.9, 58.3]52.5[50.5, 54.5]
  adjusted59.3[57.2, 61.4]59.5[54.4, 64.5]58.4[55.7, 61.1]

No SGAsN = 552N = 29N = 108
Oppositionality
  unadjusted51.4[50.4, 52.5]53.6[49.6, 57.9]51.8[49.8, 54.0]
  adjusted57.3[54.9, 59.7]60.1[54.9, 65.8]56.7[53.7, 59.8]
Inattention
  unadjusted51.5[50.6, 52.4]51.9[48.8, 55.3]51.4[49.6, 53.3]
  adjusted55.3[53.0, 57.6]55.5[51.4, 59.9]55.2[52.5, 58.0]
Hyperactivity
  unadjusted54.7[53.6, 55.958.3[54.6, 61.9]54.0[51.9, 56.1]
  adjusted61.6[58.9, 64.364.8[60.5, 69.2]60.1[57.1, 63.1]
ADHD Index
  unadjusted53.0[52.1, 54.0]55.7[52.1, 59.4]51.6[49.8, 53.5]
  adjusted58.6[56.3, 60.8]61.1[57.1, 65.2]56.8[54.2, 59.5]

No High MV-O or MV-DN = 407N = 25N = 83
Oppositionality
  unadjusted51.3[50.1, 52.5]53.5[48.7, 58.7]53.2[50.5, 56.0]
  adjusted57.4[54.4, 60.5]60.1[53.7, 67.2]58.7[54.9, 62.8]
Inattention
  unadjusted52.3[51.2, 53.4]51.4[48.4, 54.6]53.4[51.0, 55.9]
  adjusted57.6[54.9, 60.5]56.3[52.2, 60.8]58.7[55.1, 62.5]
Hyperactivity
  unadjusted54.7[53.5, 56.0]58.1[53.7, 62.6]55.4[53.0, 57.8]
  adjusted63.0[60.1, 65.9]65.5[60.3, 70.7]63.4[59.9, 66.8]
ADHD Index
  unadjusted53.6[52.5, 54.7]54.5[50.4, 58.6]53.5[51.0, 55.9]
  adjusted61.0[58.6, 63.5]61.6[57.2, 66.1]60.7[57.4, 64.1]

Abbreviations: MV-D (Maternal Vascular Pathology: Developmental); MV-O (Maternal Vascular Pathology: Obstructive); SGA (small for gestational age)

Weighting applied so that the analytic sample reflects the POUCH Subcohort.

Adjusted for: maternal race, education, marital status, parity, maternal smoking and depressive symptoms during pregnancy, symptoms of serious mental illness at time of child survey, child sex, and child age at survey

All mean differences were non-significant.