Environ Health PerspectEnviron. Health PerspectEnvironmental Health Perspectives0091-67651552-9924National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences16263509131091610.1289/ehp.8132ehp0113-001542ResearchHealth Effects of a Mixture of Indoor Air Volatile Organics, Their Ozone Oxidation Products, and StressFiedlerNancy12LaumbachRobert12Kelly-McNeilKathie12LioyPaul12FanZhi-Hua12ZhangJunfeng123OttenwellerJohn4Ohman-StricklandPamela3KipenHoward12Department of Environmental and Occupational Medicine, University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey–Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, Piscataway, New Jersey, USAEnvironmental and Occupational Health Sciences Institute, Piscataway, New Jersey, USAUniversity of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey–School of Public Health, Piscataway, New Jersey, USAVeterans Affairs Medical Center, East Orange, New Jersey, USAAddress correspondence to N. Fiedler, Environmental and Occupational Health Sciences Institute, UMDNJ-Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, 170 Frelinghuysen Rd., Room 210, Piscataway, NJ 08854 USA. Telephone: (732) 445-0123 Ext. 625. Fax: (732) 445-0130. E-mail: nfiedler@eohsi.rutgers.edu

The authors declare they have no competing financial interests.

11200521720051131115421548213200521720052005Publication of EHP lies in the public domain and is therefore without copyright. All text from EHP may be reprinted freely. Use of materials published in EHP should be acknowledged (for example, ?Reproduced with permission from Environmental Health Perspectives?); pertinent reference information should be provided for the article from which the material was reproduced. Articles from EHP, especially the News section, may contain photographs or illustrations copyrighted by other commercial organizations or individuals that may not be used without obtaining prior approval from the holder of the copyright.

In our present study we tested the health effects among women of controlled exposures to volatile organic compounds (VOCs), with and without ozone (O3), and psychological stress. Each subject was exposed to the following three conditions at 1-week intervals (within-subject factor): VOCs (26 mg/m3), VOCs + O3 (26 mg/m3 + 40 ppb), and ambient air with a 1-min spike of VOCs (2.5 mg/m3). As a between-subjects factor, half the subjects were randomly assigned to perform a stressor. Subjects were 130 healthy women (mean age, 27.2 years; mean education, 15.2 years). Health effects measured before, during, and after each 140-min exposure included symptoms, neurobehavioral performance, salivary cortisol, and lung function. Mixing VOCs with O3 was shown to produce irritating compounds including aldehydes, hydrogen peroxide, organic acids, secondary organic aerosols, and ultrafine particles (particulate matter with aerodynamic diameter < 0.1 μm). Exposure to VOCs with and without O3 did not result in significant subjective or objective health effects. Psychological stress significantly increased salivary cortisol and symptoms of anxiety regardless of exposure condition. Neither lung function nor neurobehavioral performance was compromised by exposure to VOCs or VOCs + O3. Although numerous epidemiologic studies suggest that symptoms are significantly increased among workers in buildings with poor ventilation and mixtures of VOCs, our acute exposure study was not consistent with these epidemiologic findings. Stress appears to be a more significant factor than chemical exposures in affecting some of the health end points measured in our present study.

building-related illnesslung functionneurobehavioralozonestresssymptomsvolatile organic compounds

Between 800,000 and 1.2 million buildings in the United States may be associated with building-related illnesses, and thus, between 30 and 70 million workers are exposed to potentially unhealthy working conditions (Kreiss 1990; Woods 1989). Mixtures of volatile organic compounds (VOCs) and ozone (O3) are prominent pollutants in indoor environments (Fan et al. 2003). In some cases, VOCs measured in office buildings are associated with complaints of mucosal irritation and non-specific symptoms such as headache (Hodgson et al. 1991; Ten Brinke et al. 1998). Furthermore, healthy men and women intentionally exposed to similar mixtures of VOCs report increased symptoms of eye, nose, and throat irritation and reduced air quality ratings relative to clean air conditions (Hudnell et al. 1992; Prah et al. 1998). The number of symptoms reported in controlled exposure studies, however, are relatively few and of mild intensity compared with the ongoing complaints of office workers (Apter et al. 1994; Mendell and Smith 1990; Nordstrom et al. 1994; Zweers et al. 1992). Thus, some investigators suggest that when O3 reacts with VOCs in building environments, secondary products including ultrafine particles (particulate matter with aerodynamic diameter < 0.1 μm)may mediate the more substantial effects found in offices (Bako-Biro et al. 2004; Fan et al. 2003; Rohr et al. 2002; Weschler and Shields 2000; Wolkoff and Nielsen 2001). Our study assesses a selected suite of subjective and objective markers in response to the following exposure conditions: VOCs, VOCs + O3, and ambient air with a 1-min spike of VOCs [masked clean air (MCA)]. We hypothesized that exposure to VOCs or VOCs + O3 would result in greater symptom severity, compromised neurobehavioral performance, reduced lung function, and increased salivary cortisol relative to the MCA exposure (hypothesis 1: exposure main effect).

Gender and psychological stress also contribute to health complaints in buildings (Crawford and Bolas 1996; Hodgson 1995; Mendell 1993). For example, Bachmann and Myers (1995) found gender and psychological symptoms to be significant predictors of symptoms in two problem and one nonproblem building. Temperature, uncomfortable humidity, and reported odors, however, were also associated with symptoms in the buildings investigated. Women consistently report the highest prevalence of symptoms (Skov et al. 1989; Stenberg and Wall 1995), although external psychological stress (work load and control) is also associated with complaints (Norback et al. 1990; Ryan and Morrow 1992). Therefore, we chose to include only women in our study and to expose subjects to chemical mixtures with and without psychological stress. We hypothesized that subjects would report significantly greater symptom severity and would show a greater cortisol response when exposed to VOCs or VOCs + O3 with psychological stress compared with these exposure conditions without stress or to the MCA condition with or without psychological stress (hypothesis 2: exposure × stress interaction).

In summary, indoor environmental quality is affected by numerous factors, including biological, chemical, and particulate pollutants; temperature and humidity; quality of the heating, ventilation, and air conditioning system; noise; light; and odor (Mendell and Heath 2005). Our present study assessed the interaction of chemical pollutants and psychological stress on subjective (i.e., symptoms) and objective (i.e., cortisol, lung function, neurobehavioral performance) indicators of health effects, while holding temperature, humidity, noise, and light constant. Furthermore, our study added an untested exposure dimension created by combining VOCs with O3, shown in previous studies to produce a suite of irritating gas and condensed-phase products (Fan et al. 2003).

Materials and MethodsSubjects

One hundred thirty healthy, nonsmoking women, who were on average 27.2 years of age (SD = 8.0) with 15.2 (SD = 1.9) years of education, were recruited via advertisements in local newspapers. The ethnic composition of the sample was as follows: Caucasian, 56% (n = 73); black, 10% (n = 13); Hispanic, 8% (n = 10); Asian, 20% (n = 26); other, 6% (n = 8). Subjects completed a medical history, physical examination, and standard clinical blood chemistry to rule out previous significant occupational exposure to chemicals and the following health conditions: neurologic disease or brain injury, stroke or cardiovascular disease, serious pulmonary disease including asthma, liver or kidney disease, serious gastrointestinal disorders, known endocrine disease, pregnancy or lactation, and major psychiatric conditions, including psychoses, bipolar disorder, alcoholism or drug abuse, and multiple chemical sensitivity with significant illness behavior or disability. Three hundred forty-one individuals were screened for the study. Fifty-four were excluded at the telephone screening because of medical conditions, 74 never came in for their appointed physical examination, 40 declined to participate after the physical examination, 19 were excluded for medical conditions discovered at the physical examination, 4 dropped out after their first exposure, and 6 dropped out after their second exposure (n = 197). Fourteen of the 144 subjects who completed the study were pilot subjects whose data were not included in the analysis.

Dependent MeasuresSymptom questionnaire.

Subjects rated each symptom on a ratio scale from 0 (barely detectable/no sensation) to 100 (strongest imaginable) (Green et al. 1996). We chose symptoms based on previous literature assessing the health effects of indoor air mixtures (Molhave et al. 1986; Prah et al. 1998). These symptoms included the cognitive and physical effects expected of VOC mixtures (Hudnell et al. 1992; Molhave et al. 1986), anxiety symptoms associated with the odor of exposure, eye irritation, upper respiratory and lower respiratory symptoms associated with O3 and the secondary products generated from the reactions of O3 with VOCs (Fan et al. 2003), and somatic symptoms not typically associated with VOC mixtures (Appendix 1) (Dalton et al. 1997).

Neurobehavioral.

This computerized divided-attention test of cognitive performance, performance on-line (POL) (Mills et al. 1996) offered five different levels of complexity. The test was validated in alcohol dosing trials and was developed explicitly for use in repeated-measures studies of alcohol and drug effects. POL included a central task in which the subject was presented with two lanes of traffic, divided by a double yellow line. Four conditions of “headlights” and “tail lights” appeared on any one trial. The subject was instructed to press the space bar only when a “safe” condition (i.e., left lane, white headlights, and right lane, red tail lights) existed. The peripheral task required the subject to respond with one of four arrow keys (up, down, left, right) in the direction of the critical stimulus (red octagon among other shapes). Task difficulty increased by increasing the number of distracting stimuli in the peripheral display to a random assortment of different colored circles, squares, and triangles. For the divided-attention display, the subject responded to both central and peripheral critical stimuli. After practice, 10 trials of 45 displays were presented at the most complex level. A composite performance score, composed of seven component scores, including hits, misses, false positives, response latency, and responses to targets at varying levels of visual angle, was the performance variable measured (Badiani et al. 1995).

Cortisol assays.

An extensive literature documents the significant (r ≥0.90) association between salivary and plasma cortisol (Kirschbaum and Hellhammer 1989, 1994). Salivary flow, which may be affected by anxiety, is not documented to affect the concentration of salivary cortisol (Dirks et al. 1988; Kahn et al. 1988), although circadian rhythm affects cortisol production (Walker et al. 1984). Thus, we tested subjects at the same time of day to control for the well-known circadian effect on cortisol production. Kirschbaum and Hellhammer (1989, 1994) also reported that, although a highly significant correlation is shown between salivary and plasma cortisol, absolute values vary significantly. However, in our study, we evaluated relative change scores rather than absolute values. Because hormonal fluctuations also affect cortisol levels, we measured salivary estradiol at baseline before each exposure session to account for any effects of ovulation on salivary cortisol.

Consistent with Kirschbaum and Helhammer (1989), we collected samples using the Salivette (Sarstedt Inc., Rommelsdorf, Germany) method. We asked the subject to chew on a cotton swab for 60 sec, then place the swab into a Salivette holder and affix the cap. The samples were centrifuged at 3,000 rpm for 10 min, which produced 0.5–1 mL of saliva. The saliva was frozen. Samples were analyzed, blind to exposure condition and subject characteristics, in one of the co-investigators’ laboratories (J.O.). Samples were run in duplicate, and equal numbers of samples from each group were run in the same assays.

Pulmonary function test.

With the subjects in standing position with nose clips attached, spirometry was performed using a Multispiro SX spirometry system (Creative Biomedics, Inc., San Clemente, CA) that was calibrated daily. We used the highest value for each parameter from any one of three reproducible tracings before exposure for comparison with the highest value for each parameter postexposure. Forced vital capacity (FVC), forced expiratory volume in 1 sec (FEV1), and forced expiratory flow 5–75% (FEF25–75) were parameters of interest.

Independent VariablesPublic speaking task stressor.

After a 4-min silent preparation period, subjects constructed and delivered a 4-min speech on the following controversial scenarios: a) construct a presentation on the causes of gray hair from a Reader’s Digest article; b) present a position on whether homosexual men should be allowed in the military and be given special civil rights protection; c) defend herself against a shoplifting charge in front of three authority figures (al’Absi et al. 1997). In a direct comparison al’Absi et al. (1997) demonstrated that these three different topics caused a similar level of stress (cortisol response). We randomized the order of the three scenarios across subjects such that each subject delivered a speech on a different topic in each of her three exposure conditions. After the preparation period, subjects were told by the experimenter to stand, face the video camera, and begin giving the speech. To enhance the stressfulness of the procedure, we told each subject that the research technician was evaluating the speech as it was given and that three staff members would evaluate a videotape of the speech later; the subject was promised an additional stipend of $10 if she performed well. After completion of the three experimental sessions, all subjects received the added stipend regardless of performance.

Controlled environment facility and exposure generation.

The Environmental and Occupational Health Sciences Institute Controlled Environment Facility (CEF) is a stainless steel room 2.2 m × high × 4.1 m wide × 2.7 m deep with a total volume of 25 m3. The air supply is treated in a series of conditioning processes, which include air cooling/heating, humidification/dehumidification, and filtration through carbon and high-efficiency particulate air (HEPA) filters. All parameter controls are computer interfaced to maintain constant conditions in the CEF. To simulate a typically ventilated office building and to allow sufficient time for the formation of O3–VOC reaction products, the air flow rate through the facility was controlled at 1.8 ± 0.2 air changes/hr for all the exposures conducted in this study. The air supply enters the facility through two diffusers in the ceiling and exits through the perforated stainless steel floor to the exhaust vents. Small brushless fans (to prevent unwanted particle generation from brush degradation) were used in the CEF to ensure that the air was well mixed. During exposure sessions, the relative humidity and temperature were controlled to a range of between 24 and 49% and 73–82°F (23–28°C), respectively. A Teflon partition separated the subjects’ work stations, which were equipped with a computer and typing stand on a stainless steel table. This configuration allowed exposure of two subjects at a time. The technician communicated separately with each subject through headphones and could view subjects at all times through a two-way window. For subject safety, an electrocardiogram (ECG) signal was collected using disposable electrodes and sampled at 996 Hz through the Flexcomp Biomonitoring system 1.51 B (Thought Technology Ltd., Montreal, Quebec, Canada). This allowed continuous monitoring of heart rate during the exposure period.

The composition and relative weight of the VOC mixture used were similar to those in previous indoor air studies (Kjaergaard et al. 1991; Molhave et al. 1986; Otto et al. 1992) and are reported in Table 1 (Fan et al. 2003). d-Limonene, the most frequently identified terpene in indoor air, was also added to the mixture. A flask containing the liquid mixture of these 23 compounds was heated to 250°C by a hot plate, flash evaporated, and injected into the clean air stream that delivered the VOC mixture into the CEF. Constant concentrations of chemical compounds can be maintained in the CEF by injection of the chemicals continuously into the air supply, which flows through the CEF without recirculation. The desired concentration of VOCs in the indoor environment was achieved by adjusting the flow rate of the delivering air using a mass flow controller.

Total VOC concentration in the air of the indoor environment was approximately 26 mg/m3, with the concentration of each compound below the threshold limit value recommended for occupational exposure by the American Conference of Governmental Industrial Hygienists (1992). MCA was generated by introducing a 1-min spike of the VOC mixture at approximately 10% of the exposure concentration for the VOC exposure condition. The maximum total VOC concentration in the CEF during the MCA condition was approximately 2.5 mg/m3. Given the air exchange rate used in the experiments, it took about 90 min for the masking concentration to decay to 0.25 mg/m3.

The background O3 concentration is < 3 ppb in the CEF. O3 generated in situ by an O3 generator was delivered into the CEF and was maintained at a steady-state concentration of 40 ppb. This was consistent with the intention to examine the effect of products of O3–VOC reactions rather than O3 itself. Table 2 compares the concentrations of byproducts generated in the VOC + O3 condition relative to the VOC condition. Exposure generation and characterization were described in detail in a previous publication (Fan et al. 2003).

Procedures

Before receiving a complete physical examination, including a medical history review and routine blood chemistries, subjects who met inclusion criteria gave written informed consent according to the University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey–Robert Wood Johnson Medical School Institutional Review Board. To reduce practice and novelty effects, subjects were shown the CEF and trained to perform the following procedures: symptom questionnaire, speech and neurobehavioral tasks, salivary sampling for cortisol and estradiol, nasal lavage, and spirometry. Subjects were randomly assigned to the order of exposure conditions, half with and half without the stressor. Subjects were told during informed consent that they may be asked to perform a public speaking task. They were not told whether they would be performing the task before any exposure session. Thus, subjects were blind to the stress condition. Neither the subject nor the research technicians were told exposure conditions.

Each experimental session was 3 hr in duration and occurred in the morning. Subjects were asked not to use caffeine or alcohol on the day before and on the day of the testing session. Subjects also could not have an active upper respiratory illness (either infection or allergy) or use medication for allergies or other respiratory conditions for 1-week before each exposure session. These conditions were queried by a nurse before participation on each day of exposure, and subjects were rescheduled if necessary.

Two subjects were tested at the same time. On the day of the first exposure session, a pregnancy test was given. Under typical room air conditions, a spirogram was performed in our clinical center before and after each exposure session. Subjects also completed the symptom questionnaire, practiced the POL, and performed baseline nasal lavage and salivary sampling for estradiol analysis in our clinical center.

We escorted subjects to the CEF, where they were seated at a table, had ECG sensors attached, and were given a pair of headphones used to communicate instructions for administration of questionnaires and tasks by the research technician. We gave each subject a loose-leaf binder with dividers to separate each of the questionnaires and tasks to be completed. Baseline measures before exposure were collected while the subject sat quietly with filtered room air, after which the exposure was administered for 140 min (Figure 1). During the initial 20 min of the exposure, subjects read quietly. We administered a 10-min vigilance task to maintain a consistent level of alertness across subjects before introduction of the psychological stressor (Jennings et al. 1992). In this simple color detection task, we instructed subjects to count bars that appeared periodically on a computer screen. After completion of the task, we asked subjects the number of bars counted. Subjects then completed either the public speaking task or simple arithmetic. While subjects performed the simple arithmetic task, they heard classical music through their headphones to screen out the other subject’s voice. After this period, all subjects typed a standard text for 10 min. A different text was used for each experimental session. As indicated in Figure 1, subjects completed questionnaires, collected salivary samples for cortisol analysis, and performed the neurobehavioral task during clean air baseline and the exposure period. Immediately after exposure we escorted subjects to our clinical center, where they performed postexposure spirometry, nasal lavage, the neurobehavioral task, and completed a questionnaires (Figure 1).

Statistical AnalysisSymptoms.

We analyzed the effects of exposure, stress, and time on both presence/absence of symptoms and symptom severity. For presence/absence, if a subject reported any symptom at all, then a “yes” was recorded; otherwise, a “no” was recorded. For presence/absence, a hierarchical logistic regression (Davidian and Giltinan 1995; McCulloch and Searle 2000) modeled the log-odds of no symptoms being reported for assessment at minute 15 (baseline) through minute 185 (after removal from exposure facility). For symptom severity, data were analyzed using a hierarchical Poisson regression model. In both cases generalized estimating equations were used that accounted for correlations between repeated measurements on the same individual (Liang and Zeger 1986). Tests of the exposure effects were conducted using type 3 score tests (Liang and Zeger 1986) of the interaction between exposure and time. Time was entered into the model as a categorical variable. We used contrasts to test whether individual changes in symptoms from baseline (minute 15) to each subsequent time point differed between exposures. The mean odds of reporting symptoms or the mean severity of symptoms at baseline was assumed to be the same for all three exposures. This analysis was first completed for the total symptoms and then for each classification of symptoms: VOC physical, VOC cognitive, eye irritation, anxiety, upper respiratory, lower respiratory, and somatic control symptoms. Results were based on the 130 subjects who received all three exposures, including the MCA condition. Uncorrected α-values are reported, with the αlevel after Bonferroni correction noted for each group of multiple comparisons.

Cortisol analyses.

Cortisol is normally elevated during ovulation, potentially attenuating a woman’s cortisol response to a stressor at that time. Therefore, cortisol data were analyzed both with and without data from any specific exposure session in which the subject’s estradiol level was ≥5 ng/mL. Based on this cutoff, 13 of the 130 (10%) subjects were ovulating during their MCA exposure, 13 of 130 (10%) were ovulating during the VOC exposure, and 15 of 130 (11.5%) were ovulating during the VOC + O3 exposure. On comparison of the cortisol data analyses with and without ovulation sessions, no differences in outcome were noted. Therefore, sessions in which subjects were ovulating were included in the final analyses of cortisol.

Given the continuous response, SAS PROC MIXED (SAS Institute Inc. 1999) was used to analyze the fixed-factor effects. The cortisol response was right skewed and therefore, for the linear models, was transformed using a square root transformation to better satisfy the normality assumption required for the mixed linear model.

Neurobehavioral performance and lung function.

A mixed linear model was used to test the effect of exposure on the composite score from the neurobehavioral task and on indicators of lung function.

ResultsHypothesis 1: Exposure Main EffectSymptoms.

After controlling for baseline symptoms (minute 15), the overall test of the exposure main effect on total symptom severity did not confirm hypothesis 1 (Figure 2). However, marginal effects for presence/absence of the subscales of VOC physical symptoms (chi squared = 18.01; df = 10; p < 0.05) and severity of lower respiratory symptoms (chi squared = 16.92; df = 10; p < 0.08) were observed; no effects were observed for the other categories of symptoms.

Neurobehavioral performance, salivary cortisol, and lung function.

There was no significant main effect of exposure on neurobehavioral performance (F = 1.00, df = 6, 387; p = 0.4273), salivary cortisol (F = 0.40; df = 6, 316; p = 0.888), or lung function (data not shown for neurobehavioral or cortisol measures; see Table 3 for lung function).

Hypothesis 2: Exposure × Stressor InteractionSymptoms.

Hypothesis 2 was not confirmed for presence/absence or for total symptom severity. However, regardless of exposure condition, subjects who were in the stress condition reported significantly greater severity on the anxiety sub-scale (chi squared = 22.73; df = 5; p < 0.0004) than those who were not. Specifically, relative to baseline either before (minute 15) or after exposure onset (minutes 60 and 90), symptoms of anxiety were significantly more severe after the stressor at minutes 110 and 125 (Figure 3). No other symptom subscale was significantly affected by stress or by the exposure × stressor interaction.

Salivary cortisol.

The hypothesized interaction effect of exposure × stress on cortisol was not significant, but the main effect of stress on cortisol was significant (F = 4.90; df = 3, 347; p = 0.0024). The significance of the main effect of stress was due to changes in cortisol levels from before the stressor (minute 90) to after the stressor (minute 125) (t = 4.00; p < 0.0001). When examining changes from minutes 110 to 125, on average, the cortisol levels decreased (from 0.178 to 0.154) for the no-stressor condition and increased slightly (from 0.140 to 0.147) for thestressor condition (t = 3.63; p = 0.0003) (Figure 4).

Discussion

The most striking result of our study was the lack of significant subjective or objective health effects from exposure to mixtures of VOCs both with and without O3, despite significant differences in the chemical composition of the air for the three conditions (Table 2). Numerous epidemiologic studies suggest that symptoms are significantly increased among workers in buildings with poor ventilation and mixtures of VOCs (Mendell 1993; Mendell et al. 2002; Sieber et al. 1996), many of which likely contain low levels of O3. Our present controlled acute exposure to similar chemical mixtures in young women, however, failed to support these epidemiologic findings. Relative to ambient air masked with a pulse of VOCs, neither the VOC nor the VOC + O3 exposures caused significantly increased symptom reports, changes in cortisol, reduced neurobehavioral performance, or changes in lung function. Subjects were more likely to report some VOC physical symptoms in the VOC and VOC + O3 conditions relative to the MCA condition. However, these effects were of marginal significance and became nonsignificant with appropriate correction for multiple comparisons. Conversely, although stress did not exacerbate exposure effects, stress significantly increased symptoms of anxiety. The effect of stress was further validated by the significant difference in cortisol for those who received the stressor relative to subjects who did not.

There was a marginal (p < 0.08) increase in severity, but not incidence, of lower respiratory symptoms with the VOC and VOC + O3 exposure. However, there were no significant changes in the lung function parameters attributable either to the VOC or VOC + O3 exposure. This is consistent with work showing increased lower respiratory symptoms at 50 mg/m3 but not at 25 mg/m3 of VOCs alone, but no change in spirometry or increase in inflammatory mediators from induced sputum at either VOC concentration, again without O3 (Pappas et al. 2000). Although rodent bioassays had indicated that relatively higher concentrations of limonene–O3 oxidation products were irritating to the respiratory tract, at our exposure concentrations we did not see an effect on lung function and only a marginal effect on symptoms (Rohr et al. 2002; Wilkins et al. 2001). Indicators of nasal inflammation were also negative for our exposures (Laumbach et al., in press). In contrast to our present findings, Kleno and Wolkoff (2004) reported increases in eye blink frequency among a small number of subjects with eye exposure only to limonene oxidation products and nitrate radicals relative to clean air. Although we did not measure eye blink frequency, symptoms of eye irritation were not significantly greater in the VOC + O3 condition.

Our present findings regarding neurobehavioral performance are consistent with those of Otto et al. (1992), who reported no changes in neurobehavioral performance among subjects exposed to a similar mixture of 23 VOCs relative to clean air. However, the research group from the Technical University of Denmark reported several studies in which subjects were exposed to off-gassing from a 20-year-old carpet and showed reductions in productivity on tasks that simulate office work (typing, calculations) (Wargocki et al. 1999, 2000a). Although the total VOCs were of similar concentrations between the exposure conditions with and without the carpet (~ 2.34 ppm), the composition of the chemical mixture differed substantially between the two conditions, and likely contained unidentified compounds associated with emissions from the carpet. In contrast to the negative findings in our present study, Wargocki et al. (1999, 2000a) also reported more symptoms (headache) and slower typing speed in response to the condition with the old carpet (polluted condition). Because the total VOC concentration (2.35 ppm) was less than in our present study, symptom and performance differences could be ascribed to differences in the chemical composition, to unspecified biological components, or to differences in task demands. Neither acetone nor acetic acid was present in the VOC mixture used in our present study (only small amounts were produced from the O3 and VOC reactions), but such a specific chemical effect seems unlikely. Wargocki et al. (1999, 2000b) also suggested that simulated work tasks (typing, calculations) performed over a longer period of time (265 min) than in our present study were more sensitive to the effects of poor air quality than are standard neurobehavioral tasks. Thus, although the POL was given for approximately 20 min on two occasions during exposure, this did not require the sustained effort needed to perform continuous typing for 47 min on two occasions as in the Wargocki et al. (1999, 2000b) studies. However, the use of an old carpet as an exposure was ultimately not comparable with the present specific VOC mixture.

Although some increased symptoms were observed in previous controlled exposures using similar “indoor air” mixtures (Hudnell et al. 1992; Prah et al. 1998), actually only a few out of many symptoms assessed in those studies were significantly increased. A careful examination of those symptoms exacerbated by exposure reveals some consistency with our present findings. Prah et al. (1998) reported that relative to clean air, mixtures of VOCs increased ratings of nasal irritation, odor intensity, and air quality but not health (cough, sore throat) and cognitive symptoms (memory loss, dizziness). Similarly comparing responses of a VOC mixture with clean air, Hudnell et al. (1992) reported significantly reduced air quality ratings and increased odor level, but they also reported increased symptoms of headache, eye irritation, drowsiness, and throat irritation. However, Hudnell et al. (1992) did not control for multiple statistical comparisons among the individual tests of 22 symptoms (p-value set at < 0.05). Furthermore, no previous indoor air study has “masked” the clean air condition to control for the effects of odor on symptoms. Several studies suggest that when subjects rate air contaminated with various combinations of limonene, O3, and office products (paper), they report dissatisfaction with air quality (Knudsen et al. 2002; Tamas et al., in press). However, these studies did not measure health symptoms.

To further clarify our findings, we conducted power calculations to estimate the size of the effect that would be necessary to detect a change between symptoms reported at baseline (minute 15) and either minute 60 or 125. Based on 130 subjects receiving all three exposures, we calculated the minimum actual symptom difference between the VOC and VOC + O3 exposures and the MCA exposure that would be needed to attain a power of 90%. In these power calculations, we assumed that the size of the exposure effect, relative to MCA, was the same for VOCs and VOCs + O3. To simplify the calculations, we used changes from baseline as the response variable in a repeated-measures analysis of variance. We calculated the variance components for each response and time point from the existing data. To account for multiple testing, the Bonferroni corrected significance level was used corresponding to the level used for testing in our present study.

The results indicate that in all cases, minimum average increases of between 0.6 and 3.0 points in the average score for each category of symptoms would have to exist in order to detect a difference with a power of 90% (Table 4). With symptom severity scored on a scale of 0 to 100, this indicates that very small changes were detectable with the sample size used in our study.

We performed similar power calculations for the main effect of exposure on neurobehavioral performance, cortisol, and lung function. The minimum detectable difference in neurobehavioral performance between the VOC, VOC + O3, and MCA conditions for a power of 90% was calculated at 1.30 using a 0.05 significance level. Because of a within-session learning effect, the MCA exposure resulted in a mean increase from baseline (baseline mean = 32.72; SD = 9.1) of 2.10 points on the neurobehavioral task. With 130 subjects we could detect a relative reduction of 1.30 points resulting in 0.80 point improvement for the VOC or VOC + O3 exposure conditions (i.e., 2.10 – 1.30 = 0.80). For cortisol, the minimum detectable difference in changes from baseline needed for a power of 90% was 0.066 μg/dL using a 0.05 significance level. The MCA condition resulted in a mean decrease from baseline (the average baseline = 0.2627; SD = 0.1831) of 0.1148 μg/dL. Thus, a decrease of no more than 0.0488 μg/dL in the VOC or VOC + O3 conditions was needed to detect a difference. Finally, for lung function, the minimum detectable differences in changes from baseline are 109.7 mL, 122.8 mL, and 0.917 L/sec, respectively, for changes in FEV1, FVC, and FEF25–75. These were calculated with a significance level of p = 0.0167 (0.05/3). Thus, for all dependent measures and the present sample size, relatively small changes were needed to detect a difference between exposure conditions.

Conclusions

In conclusion, relatively brief, one-time exposures to mixtures of VOCs or VOCs and their oxidation products, at the upper bound of typical indoor concentration range, did not appear to cause significant acute changes in symptoms, neurobehavioral performance, or lung function in healthy women. In contrast, the psychological stressor was effective in producing increased autonomic arousal, as indicated by salivary cortisol, and in causing increased symptoms of anxiety, both well-documented effects of psychological stress. However, stress and exposure were neither synergistic nor additive in their effects on symptoms or neurobehavioral performance. The effect of stress, however, was isolated to symptoms of anxiety and did not generalize to other more typical symptoms associated with poor indoor air such as nasal irritation or headache. Although the irritation potency of complex and variable mixtures of VOCs and their oxidation products in buildings is difficult to predict, reported air concentrations of VOCs in buildings with poor indoor air quality are typically an order of magnitude lower than the VOC concentrations used in this study. Thus, our results suggest that the VOC concentration alone may not be the most salient factor to account for acute health complaints. Our present results support the conclusion that 3-hr exposures to VOCs or to the reaction products of VOCs and O3 at concentrations typically found in nonindustrial buildings are unlikely to be a significant cause of acute health complaints or effects for most occupants.

Several caveats need to be considered for our present study. This study included the largest number of subjects to date and intentionally selected only women for study because of their hypothesized vulnerability to report indoor air quality symptoms. However, the extent to which our results apply to indoor air problems experienced in a work environment with many other demands and chronic exposures is problematic. Thus, the lack of health effects observed may simply be a function of the necessarily acute exposure paradigm with healthy young subjects. Conversely, the exposure concentrations were quantitatively higher than those documented in most buildings with indoor air complaints. Furthermore, work demands were modeled in our study through use of a known stressor as well as requirements for computerized neurobehavioral tasks, and the former was successful in causing autonomic arousal and symptoms of anxiety. Another attribute of our study was that all exposure conditions were conducted at a relatively high air exchange rate (~ 1.8 air exchanges/hr), whereas health complaints associated with indoor air quality often occur in buildings with poor ventilation (i.e., air exchange rates were an order of magnitude lower than the air exchange rate used in our study). Overall, this study suggested that for a 2-hr time period, psychological stress may be a more potent factor than ambient chemical mixtures in the complaints attributed to poor indoor environments.

Funding for our research was supported by a grant from the National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (R01 OH03691-01) and by National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences Center grant ES05022.

Referencesal’AbsiMBongardSBuchananTPincombGWLicinioJLovalloWR1997Cardiovascular and neuroendocrine adjustment to public speaking and mental arithmetic stressorsPsychophysiology342662759175441American Conference of Governmental Industrial Hygienists 1992. 1992–1993 Threshold Limit Values for Chemical Substances and Physical Agents and Biological Exposure Indices. Cincinnati, OH:American Conference of Governmental Industrial Hygienists.ApterABrackerAHodgsonMSidmanJLeungW-Y1994Epidemiology of the sick building syndromeJ Allergy Clin Immunol942772888077580BachmannMOMyersJE1995Influences on sick building syndrome symptoms in three buildingsSoc Sci Med402452517899936BadianiABrowmanKERobinsonTE1995Influence of novel versus home environments on sensitization to the psychomotor stimulant effects of cocaine and amphetamineBrain Res6742912987796109Bako-BiroZWargockiPWeschlerCJFangerPO2004Effects of pollution from personal computers on perceived air quality, SBS symptoms and productivity in officersIndoor Air1417818715104785CrawfordJOBolasSM1996Sick building syndrome, work factors and occupational stressScan J Work Environ Health22243250DaltonPWysockiCJBrodyMJLawleyHJ1997Perceived odor, irritation, and health symptoms following short-term exposure to acetoneAm J Ind Med315585699099358DavidianMGiltinanDM 1995. Nonlinear Models for Repeated Measurement Data. New York:Chapman & Hall.DirksHColicDHaeckelR 1988. Die Bestimmung von cortisol im Speichel und ihre klinische Anwendung [in German]. In: Speicheldiagnostik (Haeckel R, ed). Darmstadt, Germany: GIT-Verlag, 86–93.FanZLioyPWeschlerCFiedlerNKipenHZhangJ2003Ozone-initiated reactions with mixtures of volatile organic compounds under simulated indoor conditionsEnviron Sci Technol371811182112775052GreenHGDaltonPCowartBShafferGRankinKHigginsJ1996Evaluating the labeled magnitude scale for measuring sensations of taste and smellChem Senses213233348670711HodgsonM1995The sick-building syndromeOccup Med State Art Rev10167175HodgsonMJFrohligerJPermarETidwellCTravenNDOlenchockSA1991Symptoms and microenvironmental measures in nonproblem buildingsJ Occup Med335275332037908HudnellHKOttoDAHouseDEMolhaveL1992Exposure of humans to a volatile organic mixture. II: SensoryArch Environ Health4731381540000JenningsJRKamarckTStewartCEddyMJohnsonP1992Alternate cardiovascular baseline assessment techniques: vanilla or resting baselinePsychophysiology297427511461961KahnJ-PRubinowDRDavisCLKlingMPostRM1988Salivary cortisol: a practical method for evaluation of adrenal functionBiol Psychiatry233353493257706KirschbaumCHellhammerDH1989Salivary cortisol in psychobiological research: an overviewNeuropsychobiology221501692485862KirschbaumCHellhammerDH1994Salivary cortisol in psychoneuroendocrine research: recent developments and applicationsPsychoneuroendocrinology193133338047637KjaergaardSKMolhaveLPedersenOF1991Human reactions to a mixture of indoor air volatile organic compoundsAtmos Environ2514171426KlenoJWolkoffP2004Changes in eye blink frequency as a measure of trigeminal stimulation by exposure to limonene oxidation products, isoprene oxidation products and nitrate radicalsInt Arch Occup Environ Health7723524315007652KnudsenHAfshariAEkbergLLundgrenB 2002. Impact of ventilation rate, ozone and limonene on perceived air quality in offices. In: Indoor Air 2002: Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Indoor Air Quality and Climate, June 30–July 5, Monterey, CA (Levin H, Bendy G, Cordell J (ed). Vol II. Santa Cruz, CA:The International Academy of Indoor Air Sciences, 285–290.KreissK1990The sick building syndrome: where is the epidemiologic basisAm J Publ Health8011721173LaumbachRFiedlerNGardnerCLaskinDLFanZ-HZhangJ In press. Nasal effects of a mixture of volatile organic compounds and their ozone oxidation products. J Occup Environ Med.LiangKYZegerLL1986Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear modelsBiometrika731322McCullochCESearleSR 2000. Generalized, Linear, and Mixed Models. New York:John Wiley & Sons.MendellMSmithA1990Consistent pattern of elevated symptom rates in air-conditioned office buildings: a re-analysis of epidemiologic studiesAm J Public Health80119311992400029MendellMJ1993Non-specific symptoms in office workers: a review and summary of the epidemiologic literatureIndoor Air3227236MendellMJFiskWJKreissKLevinHAlexanderDCainWS2002Improving the health of workers in indoor environments: priority research needs for a national occupational research agendaAm J Public Health921430144012197969MendellMJHeathGA2005Do indoor pollutants and thermal conditions in schools influence student performance? A critical review of the literatureIndoor Air15275215660567MillsKCParkmanKMSpruillSE1996A PC-based software test for measuring alcohol and drug effects in human subjectsAlcohol Clin Exp Res20158215918986207MolhaveLBackBPedersenOF1986Human reactions to low concentrations of volatile organic compoundsEnviron Int12167175NorbackDIngegerdMWidstromJ1990Indoor air quality and personal factors related to the sick building syndromeScan J Work Environ Health16121128NordstromKNorbackDAkselssonR1994Effect of air humidification on the sick building syndrome and perceived indoor air quality in hospitals: a four month longitudinal studyOccup Environ Med516836888000493OttoDHudnellHKHouseDMolhaveLCountsW1992Exposure of humans to a volatile organic mixture, behavioral assessmentArch Environ Health4723301539999PappasGPHerbertRJHendersonWKoenigJStoverBBarnhartS2000The respiratory effects of volatile organic compoundsInt J Occup Environ Health61810637531PrahJDCaseMWGoldsteinGM19981998 equivalence of sensory responses to single and mixed volatile organic compounds at equimolar concentrationsEnviron Health Perspect1067397449799190RohrACWilkinsCKClausenPAHammerMNielsenGDWolkoffP2002Upper airway and pulmonary effects of oxidation products of (+)-alpha-pinene, d-limonene, and isoprene in BALB/c miceInhal Toxicol1466368412122569RyanCMMorrowKA1992Dysfunctional buildings or dysfunctional people: an examination of the sick building syndrome and allied disordersJ Consult Clin Psychol602202241592951SAS Institute Inc 1999. SAS/STAT User’s Guide, Version 8. 1999. Cary, NC:SAS Institute Inc.SieberWKPetersenMRStaynerL1996The NIOSH indoor environmental evaluation experience: part three—associations between environmental factors and self-reported health outcomesAppl Occup Environ Hyg1113871392SkovPValbjornOPedersenBVDanish Indoor Climate Study Group1989Influence of personal characteristics, job-related factors and psychosocial factors on the sick building syndromeScan J Work Environ Health15286295StenbergBWallS1995Why do women report “sick building symptoms” more often than menSoc Sci Med404915027725123TamasGWeschlerCJToftumJFangerPO In press. The influence of ozone-limonene reactions on perceived air quality. Indoor Air.Ten BrinkeJSelvinSHodgsonATFiskWJMendellCPKoshlandCP1998Development of new volatile organic compound exposure metrics and their relationship to “sick building syndrome” symptomsIndoor Air8140152WalkerRFJoyceBGDyasJ 1984. Immunoassays of steroids in saliva. In: Salivary Cortisol. 1: Monitoring Changes in Normal Adrenal Activity (Read GF, Riad-Fahmy D, Walker RF, eds). Cardiff, Wales:Alpha Omega, 308–316.WargockiPWyonDPBaikYKClausenGFangerPO1999Perceived air quality, sick building syndrome (SBS) symptoms, and productivity in an office with two different pollution loadsIndoor Air916517910439554WargockiPWyonDPFangerP 2000a. Productivity is affected by the air quality in offices. In: Proceedings of Healthy Buildings 2000, 6–10 August 2000, Helsinki, Finland (Sappanen O, Sateri J, eds). Vol 1. Oy, Finland:SIY Indoor Air Information, 635–640.WargockiPWyonDPSundellJClausenGFangerP2000bThe effects of outdoor supply rate in an office on perceived air quality, sick building syndrome (SBS) symptoms, and productivityIndoor Air1022223611089327WeschlerCJShieldsHC2000The influence of ventilation on reactions among indoor pollutants: modeling and experimental observationsIndoor Air109210011980107WilkinsCKClausenPAWolkoffPLarsenSTHammerMLarsenK2001Formation of strong airway irritants in mixtures of isoprene/ozone and isoprene/ozone/nitrogen dioxideEnviron Health Perspect10993794111673123WolkoffPNielsenGD2001Organic compounds in indoor air—their relevance for perceived indoor air quality?Atmos Environ3544074417WoodsJE1989Cost avoidance and productivity in owning and operating buildingsOccup Med State Art Rev4753770ZweersTPrellerLBrunekreefBBoleijJSM1992Health and indoor climate complaints of 7043 office workers in 61 buildings in the NetherlandsIndoor Air2127136Appendix 1. Symptom list
VOC physical
 Headache
 Fatigue
 Lightheaded
 Drowsy
 Nausea
VOC cognitive
 Difficulty concentrating
 Disoriented/confused
 Dizzy
Eye irritation
 Eye irritation (burning, dryness, or itching)
 Runny/watery eyes
Anxiety
 Feel jittery in body
 Feel nervous
 Heart palpitations
 Feel tense
 Worried
Upper respiratory
 Sneeze
 Nasal congestion
 Choking
 Throat irritation (burning or dryness)
 Nose irritation, dryness, or itching
Lower respiratory
 Short of breath
 Wheezy
 Chest tightening
 Chest pain
 Coughing
Somatic control
 Skin irritation or dryness
 Stomachache
 Numbness/tingling
 Ear ringing
 Leg cramps
 Back pain
 Sweating
 Body aches

Time line for experimental procedure. Abbreviations: Cort1, cortisol collection at minute 15; Cort2, cortisol collection at minute 90; Cort3, cortisol collection at minute 110; Cort 4, cortisol collection at minute 130; Neuro1, neurobehavioral task (POL) at minute 15; Neuro2, Neurobehavioral task (POL) at minute 60; Neuro3, neurobehavioral task (POL) at minute 140; Neuro4, neurobehavioral task (POL) at 5 min postexposure; Sym1, symptom questionnaire at baseline; Sym2, symptom questionnaire at minute 15; Sym3, symptom questionnaire at minute 60; Sym4, symptom questionnaire at minute 90; Sym5, symptom questionnaire at minute 110; Sym6, symptom questionnaire at minute 130; Sym7, symptom questionnaire at 5 min postexposure.

Total mean symptom severity at each time point across exposures.

Mean anxiety symptom severity at each time point across exposures stress versus no stress. Bonferroni correction: p < 0.003. Time point comparison for anxiety symptom sensitivity is as follows: change from minute 15 (baseline) to minute 110 (poststress), χ2 = 15.17 (p < 0.0001); change from minute 60 (after exposure onset) to minute 110 (poststress), χ 2 = 12.52 (p < 0.0004); change from minute 90 (prestress) to minute 110 (poststress), χ 2 = 30.16 (p < 0.0001); change from minute 110 to minute 125, χ2 = 22.29 (p < 0.0001); change from minute 110 to minute 185 (poststress), χ 2 = 11.64 (p < 0.0006).

Cortisol over time for stress versus no stress regardless of exposure.

The mixture of VOCs.

No.CompoundRelative weightConcentration (mg/m3)
1n-Butylacetate108.25
2p-Xylene108.25
3n-Butanol10.825
4n-Decane10.825
51-Decene10.825
61,1-Dichloroethane10.825
7d-Limonene10.825
8Ethylbenzene10.825
9Ethoxyethylacetate10.825
10n-Hexanal10.825
11n-Hexane10.825
12n-Nonane10.825
13α -Pinene10.825
142-Butanone0.10.083
15Cyclohexane0.10.083
163-Methyl-2-butanone0.10.083
174-Methyl-2-pentanone0.10.083
18n-Pentanal0.10.083
19Isopropanol0.10.083
20n-Propylbenzene0.10.083
211,2,4-Trimethylbenzene0.10.083
22n-Undecane0.10.083
231-Octene0.010.008
Total26.330

Summary of products observed during different exposure conditions.

Exposure conditionsUltrafine particle number concentration (particles/cm3)aParticle mass concentration (μg/m3)bFormaldehyde (μg/m3)bp-Tolualdehyde (μg/m3)bGlyoxal (μg/m3)bHydrogen peroxide (ppb)a,c
MCANAd< 57NDNDNAd
VOCs (23 VOCs)2,5003–713NDND0.3
VOCs + O3 (O3 + 23 VOCs)46,000140406.24.61.9

ND, not detected.

Two-hour average concentration.

Four-hour average concentration.

Includes hydrogen peroxide and organic hydroperoxides.

Not measured but expected to be the same or lower than the 23 VOC-only condition.

Spirometry changes after exposures to MCA, VOCs, and VOCs + O3 (mean difference ± SD; n = 130).

MCAVOCsVOCs ± O3p-Value
Change in FEV1 (mL)–50.46 ± 174.026.15 ± 176.56–25.30 ± 210.330.05
Change in FVC (mL)–71.54 ± 189.22–42.62 ± 184.40–60.38 ± 237.110.52
Change in FEF25–75 (L/sec)–0.00 ± 0.410.51 ± 3.630.09 ± 0.530.12

Difference is the post-minus preexposure score. Bonferroni correction: p < 0.02.

Effect sizes for symptom severity based on 130 subjects.

VOC
Respiratory
Change (from 15 min)Total symptomaGeneralCognitiveEyeAnxietyUpperLowerSomatic
60 min0.992.692.482.071.431.420.741.04
125 min1.152.982.692.151.851.490.641.13

Calculated at a significance level of 0.05. The effect sizes for all remaining subcategories of symptoms were calculated at a significance level of 0.05/7 = 0.0071.